Lin, Hsiao, & Chen (1999) used the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) to study sustained attention development in primary school aged children. The CPT measures sustained attention deficits caused by cognitive inhibition and has been used in the past to measure sustained attention deficits in children with ADHD as well as those at risk for schizophrenia. The reasoning behind this is that in those at high risk for schizophrenia, there will be certain brain regions that are not being activated during the CPT. Previous CPT studies have been rather limited in scope and therefore this study was useful as it tested a large sample of children of both genders. They found that CPT performance improves in children between the ages of 6 and 15, particularly between the ages of 6 and 12. This finding suggests that the cognitive inhibition aspect of sustained attention develops during this age range.
Carriere et al. (2010) studied a larger age span
(20-70) to examine sustained attention over age and time. By looking across decades, they were able to
find that both SART errors and response speed decreased with age in an overall
linear trend (see Figure 1). More specifically, they found that measures of
task disengagement on the SART decreased rapidly in early adulthood (~3rd
decade), following which they remained rather stable despite the increasing of
age (See Figure 2). The differences
between that of Figure 1 and Figure 2 suggest that SART error and RT (Figure 1)
explain different aspects of performance that that of the task disengagement measured
in this study (RTCT, anticipations, & omissions; Fig. 2).
The findings on task disengagement suggest
that sustained ability does not change as a function of aging but rather as a
function of maturation early in adulthood but then remains stable with further
aging. The decreased response time of
older adults may reflect the use of a more strategic response style that leads
to a reduction in SART errors. There are a couple of possibilities for why this
might be the case. According to Jackson & Balota (2011), older adults are higher in conscientiousness compared to
younger adults. This could have an
impact this difference of mind-wandering seen in age difference. Carriere et al. conclude with an important
point, which is that had they not chosen such a large age range and looked only
at those in the second decade compared to the seventh, for example, it would
have looked as if sustained attention continuously improves with age. However, by looking across each decade, we
are able to see that it actually remains generally unchanged following the 3rd
decade.
Using versions of SART followed by a probed reading comprehension task (noting that this was a more realistic measure), Jackson & Balota (2011) found that not only do older adults not mind
wander more than younger adults, they actually tend to mind wander less. When using an
easier version of the SART, older adults responded at a slower pace and were more accurate than younger adults.
In addition, older adults had an increase in response time following an
error, which indicates that they had an increased difficulty reengaging in the
task after making an error. Older adults were higher in conscientiousness while
they also rated the tasks as both more difficult and more interesting than
younger adults. Interest in task may be
an important predictor of performance. The researchers do comment that for older adults, as opposed to college students, conducting this novel experimental context on a college campus may contribute to their reported decreased mind-wandering. I found this point interesting as I would expect that this could actually have an opposite effect, in that perhaps bringing these older adults onto a college campus could create a nostalgic effect in which they mind wander more, to their experiences when in college. This however would have caused an increase in mind-wandering. Another thought that this point then brought me to is the idea that perhaps the participation itself in a psychological study is a novel experience for older adults, whereas nowadays it is so common to conduct studies using undergraduate students, that these undergraduate participants may participate in many of these experiments and therefore may not find the act of participating as exciting as older adults new to such an experience may find it. If so, this aspect of participating in an experiment may contribute to why older adults reported the task as more interesting. If this is the case, it would be expected that older adults would generally rate tasks as more interesting/engaging over a wider variety of tasks compared to younger adults. However, regardless of what it was that made the experiment more engaging and interesting for older adults, the idea that the extent of task interest may be an important age-related difference in understanding observed effects.
In
comparing their work to that of Carriere et al. (2010) Jackson & Balota
note an important point that “…their study used a version of the SART
consistent with Robertson et al. (1997), and thus can only be directly compared
with Experiment 1 of the present study” (p.116). I think this is a very important point
because it is easy to just compare the findings of different studies on mind
wandering, however, it is important to take into account the actual methods
used in such studies which could account for any discrepancies between
findings.
No comments:
Post a Comment