This blog post discuses a variety
of studies that examine the effects that mind wandering has in our everyday
life. While Baird
et al. (2012) examine its potential benefits for future creative problem
solving, the majority of studies focus on the negative consequences of mind
wandering. Whether it affects memory (Delaney et al.,
2012), reading comprehension (McVay & Kane,
2011), or overall happiness (Killingsworth &
Gilbert, 2010), I believe there has to be some reason why we so often mind
wander, whether or not it is done consciously. Therefore, while for the most
part, these studies focus on negative implications seen in failures of
engagement with the present environment, whether we mind wander to plan our future,
understand our past, or defend ourselves from the present, there must be some
silver lining.
While much research has focused on
negative effects of mind wandering, Baird et al. (2012) examine mind wandering’s
role in facilitating creative problem solving during breaks or incubation
periods, using the Unusual Uses Task (UUT).
All participants solved UUT problems before an incubation periods, after
which participants were shown both repeated UTT (repeated-exposure) problems
and new UUT (new-exposure) problems. The
incubation period consisted of one of 4 possible conditions: a demanding task,
undemanding task, rest condition, or no condition. As predicted, those in the undemanding
task condition reported significant improvements in UUT uniqueness scores for
the repeated-exposure problems compared to all other conditions (See
blue circle marked in Figure 1), while there was no reported difference in
uniqueness scores across all conditions on the new-exposure problems. Because creativity improvement was limited to
repeated-exposure problems, suggests that engaging in a task that facilitates
mind wandering increases previous creative problem solving but not creativity
in general. It was surprising that those
in the rest condition (just sitting quietly during the incubation period)
showed no creativity increase for repeated-exposure problems (See
red circle marked in Figure 1). It
would be interesting to see what these subjects were mind wandering about
because, unlike those in the undemanding task, their mind wandering did not
facilitate UUT uniqueness scores. These results may show the importance of
undemanding tasks in facilitating the unconscious mind wandering associated
with the increase in creative problem solving. Perhaps the control group
disassociated completely from the experiment and, instead, engaged in more
personally relevant mind wandering compared to the undemanding task condition
who were still in an “experiment-mindset,” as they were engaged in an
experimental task, causing them to focus on the UUT problems in mind wandering.
Delaney et al. (2012) study the impairments or amnesic
effects associated with daydreaming based on a context-change approach. They predict that increasing distance of
thought-content (both temporally and physically) from the present environment
will increase forgetting of just-learned information due to a reduced
effectiveness of associated retrieval cues. In Experiment 1 (See Figure 1), as
predicted, the far-change condition (thought of last time visited parents’
home) showed a significant impairment in remembering words presented
immediately prior to daydreaming compared to both the near-change condition
(thought of last time at own home) and the control condition (instead of
daydreaming, told to quickly read a passage). It is interesting to note there
was no significant difference between that remembered by the near-change and
control conditions. Does this insinuate
that a similar mind wandering experience was occurring in both conditions,
therefore suggesting that we typically mind wander in a near-time/near-distance
frame of mind? This might make sense in
relation to the view that we mind wander to personally relevant goals, as it
would suggest that we prioritize what is most near. In addition, findings showed that the longer
it had been since participants had visited their parents’ house, the fewer words
remembered from List 1 (those presented before daydreaming). While this finding supports the main
hypothesis, I wonder if there is also a possible correlation between feelings
associated with one’s home life and the time when they last visited. If so, perhaps those who hadn’t visited for a
relatively long time had stronger and more negative feelings associated with
their parents’ home compared to those that visited more recently. These elicited conflicting emotions could
affect the content and strength of associated daydreaming.
When examining distance
(physically), as predicted, the far-change condition (think of an international
vacation) reported more memory impairment of List 1 words compared to other
conditions. While researchers examined the effect of distance to List 1 words
remembered (See Figure
4), they did not test for a time effect. The study extended to include
subjects that vacationed anytime within the last 3 years. I think that time
since vacation is a crucial factor that should have been measured, especially
given the significant time effect found in the previous experiment.
Overall, while these findings are
compelling and arguably plausible, they are meant to explain context-change
daydreams, however, the studied “daydreams” are not consistent with what the
researchers themselves define as a daydream: “a kind of mind wandering that
involves off-task thought”(1036). Because researches directed thinking to
daydream, the thoughts generated are actually task-relevant. They also go on to say that “the more that
one’s mental context is changed by daydreaming, the more difficult it becomes
to access what one has just experienced” (1040). However, I am left wondering if there is a
difference between one’s mind drifting away from the present to a different
context on its own, as opposed to being directed to daydream of a different
context.
McVay & Kane (2011) study the
mediating role that mind wandering plays in the association between working
memory capacity (WMC) and reading comprehension, with individual differences in
attention control explaining this relationship.
They take the Control Failures x Concerns View (McVay & Kane,
2010) in which off-task thoughts are automatically generated, based on
individuals’ current concerns in addition to environmental cues, from a
continuous stream of thought. Their
findings show that low WMC is associated with increased mind wandering and poor
reading comprehension while higher WMC is associated with a decreased tendency
to mind wander and better reading comprehension. In this executive
control-failure view of main wandering, these off-task intrusions have negative
effects on performance in demanding tasks, with individuals possessing weaker
attention-control abilities (lower WMC) having an increased likelihood of
succumbing to these interfering thoughts.
These findings therefore explain, at least in part, WMC’s predictive
value for reading comprehension.
By developing an iPhone
application, Killingsworth & Gilbert (2010) created an innovative way to
study the association between mind wandering and reported levels of happiness
within a real-world context, across a diverse sample of people. Using a
probe-caught method through participants’ iPhones, researchers were able to
measure subjects during their daily activities and experiences. In this sample, participants were caught mind
wandering much more frequently than laboratory experiments have typically
suggested. This is an important finding
as it suggests substantial differences in mind wandering generated in lab versus
real-world settings, which could affect the generalizability of lab
findings. In general, subjects from this
sample reported being less happy when mind wandering throughout all activities,
despite that, for the most part, thoughts were related to pleasant topics.
Happiness was strongly related to whether mind wandering occurred in the subject’s
previous sample, while it had no significant effect on whether mind wandering
occurred in the next sample. From this, Killingsworth & Gilbert conclude
that mind wandering seems to be the cause of reported unhappiness. This study
therefore demonstrates a possible emotional cost of mind wandering.
This study is noteworthy, mainly due
to its use of technology to capture a more real-world picture of mind wandering,
which may vary significantly from laboratory studies. However, I do have reservations about some of
the researchers’ conclusions. Because mind
wandering is probe-caught, it is possible that subjects misconstrue their
reported unhappiness as a product of mind wandering, though it might actually
stem from mind wandering disturbance. The reason I propose this is because participants
often reported mind wandering about pleasant thoughts. Because happiness was measured before
questions about mind wandering were asked, it is possible that people
attributed their unhappiness to the wrong source (i.e. to mind wandering as
opposed to the probe pulling them away from their pleasant thoughts). Another possibility for these results could
be that being probed to notice one is mind wandering is in itself disturbing, leading
people feel to unhappy when they become aware they were mind wandering and had
no control over it (even though they were actually happy while mind
wandering). In addition, perhaps because
mind wandering tends to be seen as a mental weakness or failure, the mere
acknowledgement of mind wandering may make people unhappy due to a sense of
“being caught in the act.” While I am
only speculating possible effects of this probe-caught method in relation to
reported unhappiness, I remain unconvinced that it is the act of mind wandering
itself that causes unhappiness. Perhaps
it is not that “a wandering mind is an unhappy mind,”(932) but rather that “a
wandering mind caught is an unhappy
mind.”
No comments:
Post a Comment